The second example of the unpredictability of binocular effects has direct bearing on stereopsis but involves retinal rivalry, which we allude to in our discussion of strabismus in Chapter 9. You cannot fuse this pair in the way you can fuse other pairs, such as the first circle stereopairs shown earlier. Instead, you get "retinal rivalry"--a patchwork quilt of vertical and horizontal areas whose borders fade in and out and change position. If two very different images are made to fall on the two retinas, very often one will be, as it were, turned off. If you look at the left black-and-white square in this diagram with the left eye and the right one with the right eye, by crossing or uncrossing your eyes or with a stereoscope, you might expect to see a grid, or mesh, like a window screen. Actually, it is virtually impossible to see both sets of orthogonal stripes together. You may see all vertical or all horizontal, with one set coming into view for a few seconds as the other fades, or you may see a kind of patchwork mosaic of the two, in which the patches move and blend in and out from one orientation to the other, as shown by the figure to the left. For some reason the nervous system will not put up with so different simultaneous stimuli in any one part of the visual field--it suppresses one of them. But here we use the word suppress as a short way of redescribing the phenomenon: we don't really know how the suppression is accomplished or at what level in the central nervous system it takes place. To me, the patchy quality of the outcome of the battle between the two eyes suggests that the decision takes place rather early in visual processing, conceivably in area 17 or 18. (I am glad I do not have to defend such a guess.)